From ":MARIJUANA AND ACTUAL DRIVING PERFORMANCE":

           U.S. Department of Transportation,
           National Highway Traffic Safety Administration
           (DOT HS 808 078), Final Report, November 1993:

           "This program of research has shown that marijuana, when taken alone, produces a moderate degree of
           driving impairment which is related to the consumed THC dose. The impairment manifests itself mainly in the
           ability to maintain a steady lateral position on the road, but its magnitude is not exceptional in comparison
           with changes produced by many medicinal drugs and alcohol. Drivers under the influence of marijuana retain
           insight in their performance and will compensate, where they can, for example, by slowing down or increasing
           effort. As a consequence, THC's adverse effects on driving performance appear relatively small."

           ABSTRACT

           Marijuana's effects on actual driving performance were assessed in a series of three studies wherein
           dose-effect relationships were measured in actual driving situations that progressively approached reality.
           The first was conducted on a highway closed to other traffic. Subjects (24) were treated on separate
           occasions with THC 100, 200 and 300 g/kg, and placebo. They performed a 22-km road tracking test
           beginning 30 and 90 minutes after smoking. Their lateral position variability increased significantly after
           each THC dose relative to placebo in a dose-dependent manner for two hours after smoking. The second
           study was conducted on a highway in the presence of other traffic. Subjects (16) were treated with the same
           THC doses as before. They performed a 64-km road tracking test preceded and followed by 16-km car
           following tests. Results confirmed those of the previous study. Car following performance was only slightly
           impaired. The third study was conducted in high-density urban traffic. Separate groups of 16 subjects were
           treated with 100 g/kg THC and placebo; and, ethanol (mean BAC .034 g%) and placebo. Alcohol impaired
           performance relative to placebo but subjects did not perceive it. THC did not impair driving performance yet
           the subjects thought it had. These studies show that THC in single inhaled doses up to 300 g/kg has
           significant, yet not dramatic, dose-related impairing effects on driving performance.

           INTRODUCTION

           This article describes the results of a research program that was set up to determine the dose-response
           relationship between marijuana and objectively and subjectively measured aspects of real world driving; and
           to determine whether it is possible to correlate driving performance impairment with plasma concentrations
           of the drug or a metabolite. The program consisted of three driving studies in which a variety of driving tasks
           were employed, including: maintenance of a constant speed and lateral position during uninterrupted highway
           travel, following a leading car with varying speed on a highway, and city driving. A laboratory study preceded
           the driving studies for identifying the highest THC dose to be administered in the subsequent studies.

           GENERAL PROCEDURES

           Subjects in all studies were recreational users of marijuana or hashish, i.e., smoking the drug more than once
           a month, but not daily. They were all healthy, between 21 and 40 years of age, had normal weight and
           binocular acuity, and were licensed to drive an automobile. Furthermore, law enforcement authorities were
           contacted, with the volunteers' consent, to verify that they had no previous arrests or convictions for drunken
           driving or drug trafficking.
           Each subject was required to submit a urine sample immediately upon arrival at the test site. Samples were
           assayed qualitatively for the following common 'street drugs' (or metabolites): cannabinoids,
           benzodiazepines, opiates, cocaine, amphetamines and barbiturates. In addition a breath sample was analyzed
           for the presence of alcohol. Blood samples were repeatedly taken after smoking by venepuncture.
           Quantitative analysis of THC and THC-COOH in plasma was performed by gas chromatography/mass
           spectrometry (gc/ms) using deuterated cannabinoids as internal standards.

           Marijuana and placebo marijuana cigarettes were supplied by the U.S. National Institute on Drug Abuse. The
           lowest and highest THC concentrations in the marijuana cigarettes used in the studies were 1.75% and
           3.57%, respectively. Subjects smoked the administered cigarettes through a plastic holder in their customary
           fashion.
           Subjects were accompanied during every driving test by a licensed driving instructor. A redundant control
           system in the test vehicle was available for controlling the car, should emergency situations arise.
           In each study, subjects repeatedly performed certain simple laboratory tests (e.g. critical instability tracking,
           hand and posture stability), estimated their levels of intoxication and indicated their willingness to drive
           under several specified conditions of urgency. In addition, heart rate and blood pressure were measured.
           Results of these measurements are reported elsewhere (Robbe, 1994).

           LABORATORY STUDY

           Methods

           Twenty-four subjects, equally comprised of men and women, participated in this study. They were allowed to
           smoke part or all of the THC content in three cigarettes until achieving the desired psychological effect. The
           only requirement was to smoke for a period not exceeding 15 minutes. When subjects voluntarily stopped
           smoking, cigarettes were carefully extinguished and retained for subsequent gravimetric estimation of the
           amount of THC consumed.

           Results

           Six subjects consumed one cigarette, thirteen smoked two and four smoked three (data from one male
           subject were excluded from the results because no drug was found in his plasma after smoking). The average
           amount of THC consumed was 20.8 mg, after adjustment for body weight, 308 g/kg. It should be noted that
           these amounts of THC represent both the inhaled dose and the portion that was lost through pyrolysis and
           side-stream smoke during the smoking process. There were no significant differences between males and
           females, nor between frequent and infrequent users, with respect to the weight adjusted preferred dose. It
           was decided that the maximum dose for subsequent driving studies would be 300 g/kg.

           STUDY 1: DRIVING ON A RESTRICTED HIGHWAY

           Methods

           The first driving study was conducted on a highway closed to other traffic. The same twelve men and twelve
           women who participated in the laboratory study served again as the subjects. They were treated on separate
           occasions with marijuana cigarettes containing THC doses of 0 (placebo), 100, 200, and 300 g/kg. Treatments
           were administered double-blind and in a counterbalanced order. On each occasion, subjects performed a
           road-tracking test beginning 40 minutes after initiation of smoking and repeated one hour later. The test
           involved maintaining a constant speed at 90 km/h and a steady lateral position between the delineated
           boundaries of the traffic lane. Subjects drove 22 km on a primary highway and were accompanied by a
           licensed driving instructor. The primary dependent variable was the standard deviation of lateral position
           (sdlp), which has been shown to be both highly reliable and very sensitive to the influence of sedative
           medicinal drugs and alcohol. Other dependent variables were mean speed, and standard deviations of speed
           and steering wheel angle. Blood samples were taken 10 minutes before the driving tests (i.e. 30 and 90
           minutes after initiation of smoking, respectively).

           Results

           All subjects were willing and able to finish the driving tests without great difficulty. Data from one male
           subject were excluded from the results because no drug was found in his plasma after smoking.
           Figure 1 [absent here] demonstrates that marijuana impairs driving performance as measured by an increase
           in lateral position variability: all three THC doses significantly affected sdlp relative to placebo<.012, .001 &
           .001, for the 100, 200 & 300 g/kg conditions, respectively. The Dose by Time effect was not significant
           indicating that impairment after marijuana was the same in both trials. Marijuana's effects on sdlp were
           compared to those of alcohol obtained in a very similar study by Louwerens et al. (1987). It appeared that the
           effects of the various administered THC doses (100-300 g/kg) on sdlp were equivalent to those associated
           with bacs in the range of 0.03-0.07 g%. Other driving performance measures were not significantly affected
           by THC. Plasma concentrations of the drug were clearly related to the administered dose and time of blood
           sampling but unrelated to driving performance impairment. STUDY 2: DRIVING ON A NORMAL
           HIGHWAY IN TRAFFIC Methods The second driving study was conducted on a highway in the presence of
           other traffic and involved both a road-tracking and a car-following test. A new group of sixteen subjects,
           equally comprised of men and women, participated in this study. A conservative approach was chosen in
           designing the present study in order to satisfy the strictest safety requirements. That is, the study was
           conducted according to an ascending dose series design where both active drug and placebo conditions were
           administered, double-blind, at each of three THC dose levels. THC doses were the same as those used in the
           previous study, namely 100, 200, and 300 g/kg. Cigarettes appeared identical at each level of treatment
           conditions. If any subject would have reacted in an unacceptable manner to a lower dose, he/she would not
           have been permitted to receive a higher dose. The subjects began the car-following test 45 minutes after
           smoking. The test was performed on a 16 km segment of the highway and lasted about 15 minutes. After the
           conclusion of this test, subjects performed a 64-km road-tracking test on the same highway which lasted
           about 50 minutes. At the conclusion of this test, they participated again in the car-following test. Blood
           samples were taken both before the first and after the last driving test (i.e. 35 and 190 minutes after initiation
           of smoking, respectively). The road-tracking test was the same as in the previous study except for its
           duration and the presence of other traffic. The car-following test involved attempting to match velocity with,
           and maintain a constant distance from a preceding vehicle as it executed a series of deceleration/acceleration
           maneuvers. The preceding vehicle's speed would vary between 80 and 100 km/h and the subject was
           instructed to maintain a 50 m distance however the preceding vehicle's speed might vary. The duration of one
           deceleration and acceleration maneuver was approximately 50 seconds and six to eight of these maneuvers
           were executed during one test, depending upon traffic density. The subject's average reaction time to the
           movements of the preceding vehicle, mean distance and coefficient of variation of distance during maneuvers
           were taken as the dependent variables from this. Results All subjects were able to complete the series
           without suffering any untoward reaction while driving. Data from one female subject were excluded from the
           results because no drug was found in her plasma after smoking. Road-tracking performance in the standard
           test was impaired in a dose-related manner by THC and confirmed the results obtained in the previous closed
           highway study (Figure 2). The 100 g/kg dose produced a slight elevation in mean sdlp, albeit not statistically
           significant (p.13). The 200 g/kg dose produced a significant (p.023) elevation, of dubious practical relevance.
           The 300 g/kg dose produced a highly significant (p<.007) elevation which may be viewed as practically
           relevant. After marijuana smoking, subjects drove with an average speed that was only slightly lower than
           after placebo and very close to the prescribed level. In the car-following test, subjects maintained a distance
           of 45-50 m while driving in the successive placebo conditions. They lengthened mean distance by 8, 6 and 2 m
           in the corresponding THC conditions after 100, 200 and 300 g/kg, respectively. The initially large
           drug-placebo difference and its subsequent decline is a surprising result. Our explanation for this observation
           is that the subjects' caution was greatest the first time they undertook the test under the influence of THC
           and progressively less thereafter. The reaction time of the subjects to changes in the preceding vehicle's
           speed increased following THC treatment, relative to placebo. The administered THC dose was inversely
           related to the change in reaction time, as it was to distance. However, increased reaction times were partly
           due to longer distance (i.e. the longer the distance to the preceding vehicle, the more difficult it is to perceive
           changes in its speed). Statistical adjustment for this confounding variable resulted in smaller and
           non-significant increases in reaction time following marijuana treatment, the greatest impairment (0.32 s)
           being observed in the first test following the lowest THC dose (Figure 3). Distance variability followed a
           similar pattern as mean distance and reaction time; the greatest impairment was found following the lowest
           dose. As in the previous study, plasma concentrations of the drug were not related to driving impairment.
           STUDY 3: DRIVING IN URBAN TRAFFIC Methods The program proceeded into the third driving study,
           which involved tests conducted in high-density urban traffic. There were logical and safety reasons for
           restricting the THC dose to 100 g/kg. It was given to a new group of 16 regular marijuana (or hashish) users,
           along with a placebo. For comparative purposes, another group of 16 regular users of alcohol, but not
           marijuana, were treated with a modest dose of their preferred recreational drug, ethanol, and again placebo,
           before undertaking the same city driving test. Both groups were equally comprised of men and women.
           Marijuana was administered to deliver 100 g/kg THC. The driving test commenced 30 minutes after
           smoking. The alcohol dose was chosen to yield a bac approaching 0.05 g% when the driving test commenced
           45 minutes after onset of drinking. Active drug and placebo conditions were administered double-blind and in
           a counterbalanced order in each group. Blood samples were taken immediately prior to and following all
           placebo and drug driving tests (i.e. 20 and 80 minutes after initiation of smoking, or 35 and 95 minutes after
           initiation of drinking). Driving tests were conducted in daylight over a constant 17.5 km route within the city
           limits of Maastricht. Subjects drove their placebo and active-drug rides through heavy, medium and low
           density traffic on the same day of the week, and at the same time of day. Two scoring methods were
           employed in the present study. The first, a 'molecular' approach adopted from Jones (1978), involved the
           employment of a specially trained observer who applied simple and strict criteria for recording when the
           driver made or failed to make each in a series of observable responses at predetermined points along a
           chosen route. The second, a 'molar' approach, required the driving instructor acting as the safety controller
           during the tests to retrospectively rate the driver's performance using a shortened version of the Royal
           Dutch Tourist Association's Driving Proficiency Test. In total, 108 items were dichotomously scored, as
           either pass or fail. Total test performance was measured by the percentage items scored as 'pass'. Subscores
           were calculated for vehicle checks, vehicle handling, traffic maneuvers, observation and understanding of
           traffic, and turning'. This method has been applied previously to show the impairing effects of alcohol and
           diazepam (De Gier, 1979; De Gier et al., 1981). Results Data from two male subjects in the marijuana group
           were excluded from the results because neither THC nor THC-COOH was found in their plasma after
           smoking. Neither alcohol nor marijuana significantly affected driving performance measures obtained by the
           molecular approach, indicating that it may be relatively insensitive to drug-induced changes. The molar
           approach was more sensitive. Table 1 shows that a modest dose of alcohol (bac=0.034 g%) produced a
           significant impairment in city driving, relative to placebo. More specifically, alcohol impaired both vehicle
           handling and traffic maneuvers. Marijuana, administered in a dose of 100 g/kg THC, on the other hand, did
           not significantly change mean driving performance as measured by this approach. Subjects' ratings of driving
           quality and effort to accomplish the task were strikingly different from the driving instructor's ratings. Both
           groups rated their driving performance following placebo as somewhat better than 'normal'. Following the
           active drug, ratings were significantly lower (35%, p.009) in the marijuana, but not (5%, ns) in the alcohol
           group. Perceived effort to accomplish the driving test was about the same in both groups following placebo.
           Following the active drug, a significant (p.033) increase in perceived effort was reported by the marijuana, but
           not the alcohol group. Thus, there is evidence that subjects in the marijuana group were not only aware of
           their intoxicated condition, but were also attempting to compensate for it. These seem to be important
           findings. They support both the common belief that drivers become overconfident after drinking alcohol and
           investigators' suspicions that they become more cautious and self-critical after consuming low doses of THC,
           as smoked marijuana. Drug plasma concentrations were neither related to absolute driving performance
           scores nor to the changes that occurred from placebo to drug conditions. With respect to THC, these results
           confirm the findings in previous studies. They are somewhat surprising for alcohol but may be due to the
           restricted range of ethanol concentrations in the plasma of different subjects. DISCUSSION The results of
           the studies corroborate those of previous driving simulator and closed-course tests by indicating that THC in
           inhaled doses up to 300 g/kg has significant, yet not dramatic, dose-related impairing effects on driving
           performance (cf. Smiley, 1986). Standard deviation of lateral position in the road-tracking test was the most
           sensitive measure for revealing THC's adverse effects. This is because road-tracking is primarily controlled
           by an automatic information processing system which operates outside of conscious control. The process is
           relatively impervious to environmental changes but highly vulnerable to internal factors that retard the flow
           of information through the system. THC and many other drugs are among these factors. When they interfere
           with the process that restricts road-tracking error, there is little the afflicted individual can do by way of
           compensation to restore the situation. Car-following and, to a greater extent, city driving performance
           depend more on controlled information processing and are therefore more accessible for compensatory
           mechanisms that reduce the decrements or abolish them entirely. THC's effects on road-tracking after doses
           up to 300 g/kg never exceeded alcohol's at bacs of 0.08 g%; and, were in no way unusual compared to many
           medicinal drugs' (Robbe, 1994; Robbe and O'Hanlon, 1995; O'Hanlon et al., 1995). Yet, THC's effects differ
           qualitatively from many other drugs, especially alcohol. Evidence from the present and previous studies
           strongly suggests that alcohol encourages risky driving whereas THC encourages greater caution, at least in
           experiments. Another way THC seems to differ qualitatively from many other drugs is that the former's
           users seem better able to compensate for its adverse effects while driving under the influence. Inter-subject
           correlation's between plasma concentrations of the drug and driving performance after every dose were
           essentially nil, partly due to the peculiar kinetics of THC. It enters the brain relatively rapidly, although with
           a perceptible delay relative to plasma concentrations. Once there, it remains even at a time when plasma
           concentrations approach or reach zero. As a result, performance may still be impaired at the time that plasma
           concentrations of the drug are near the detection limit. This is exactly what happened in the first driving
           study. Therefore an important practical implications of the study is that is not possible to conclude anything
           about a driver's impairment on the basis of his/her plasma concentrations of THC and THC-COOH
           determined in a single sample. Although THC's adverse effects on driving performance appeared relatively
           small in the tests employed in this program, one can still easily imagine situations where the influence of
           marijuana smoking might have a dangerous effect; i.e., emergency situations which put high demands on the
           driver's information processing capacity, prolonged monotonous driving, and after THC has been taken with
           other drugs, especially alcohol. Because these possibilities are real, the results of the present studies should
           not be considered as the final word. They should, however, serve as the point of departure for subsequent
           studies that will ultimately complete the picture of THC's effects on driving performance. REFERENCES De
           Gier JJ (1979) A subjective measurement of the influence of ethyl/alcohol in moderate doses on real driving
           performances. Blutalkohol, 16, 363-370. De Gier JJ, 't Hart BJ, Nelemans FA and Bergman H (1981)
           Psychomotor performance and real driving performance of outpatients receiving diazepam.
           Psychopharmacology, 73, 340-347. Jones MH (1978) Driver Performance Measures for the Safe
           Performance Curriculum. Traffic Safety Center, Institute of Safety and Systems Management, University of
           South California, Los Angeles, CA (DOT HS 803 461). Louwerens JW, Gloerich ABM, de Vries G,
           Brookhuis KA and O'Hanlon JF (1987). The relationship between drivers' blood alcohol concentration (bac)
           and actual driving performance during high speed travel. Pages 183-192 in PC Noordzij and R Roszbach,
           eds., Alcohol, Drugs and Traffic Safety. Proceedings of the 10th International Conference on Alcohol, Drugs
           and Traffic Safety. Excerpta Medica, Amsterdam. O'Hanlon JF, Vermeeren A, Uiterwijk MMC, van Veggel
           LMA and Swijgman HF (1995) Anxiolytics' effects on the actual driving performance of patients and healthy
           volunteers in a standardized test: an integration of three studies. Neuropsychobiology, 31:81-88. Robbe HWJ
           (1994). Influence of Marijuana on Driving. PhD thesis,Institute for Human Psychopharmacology, University
           of Limburg, Maastricht. Robbe HWJ and O'Hanlon JF (1995) Acute and subchronic effects of paroxetine and
           amitriptyline on actual driving, psychomotor performance and subjective assessments in healthy volunteers.
           European Neuropsychopharmacology, 5:35-42 Smiley AM (1986). Marijuana: On-road and driving simulator
           studies. Alcohol, Drugs and Driving: Abstracts and Reviews 2: 121-134. Exposing Marijuana Myths Claim
           #12 : Marijuana is a Major Cause of Highway Accidents: By Lynn Zimmer, Associate Professor of
           Sociology, Queens College, & John P Morgan, Professor of Pharmacology, City University of New York
           Medical School: The Lindesmith Center, 1995 The detrimental impact of alcohol on highway safety has been
           well documented. Marijuana's opponents claim that it, too, causes significant impairment and that any
           increase in use will lead to increased highway accidents and fatalities. THE FACTS In high doses marijuana
           probably produces driving impairment in most people. However, there is no evidence that marijuana, in
           current consumption patters, contributes substantially to the rate of vehicular accidents in America. A
           number of studies have looked for evidence of drugs in blood or urine of drivers involved in fatal crashes. All
           have found alcohol present in 50 percent or more. Marijuana has been found much less often. Furthermore,
           in the majority of cases where marijuana has been detected, alcohol has been detected as well. (*1) For
           example, a recent study sponsored by the US National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (NHTSA)
           involving analysis of nearly 2000 fatal accident cases, found 6.7 percent of drivers positive for marijuana. In
           more than two-thirds of those, alcohol was present and may have been the primary contributor to the fatal
           outcome (*2) To accurately access marijuana's contribution to fatal crashed, the positive rate among
           deceased drivers would have to be compared to the positive rate from a random sample of drivers not
           involved in fatal accidents. Since the rate of the past-month marijuana use for Americans above the legal
           driving age is about 12 percent, on any given day a substantial proportion of all drivers would test positive,
           particularly since marijuana's metabolites remain in blood and urine long after its psychoactive effects are
           finished. A recent study found that one-third of those stopped for "bad driving" between the hours of 7 p.m
           and 2 a.m - mostly young males - tested positive for marijuana only. (*3). To be meaningful, these test results
           would have to be compared to those from a matched control group of drivers. A number of driving simulator
           studies have shown that marijuana does not produce the kind of psychomotor impairment evident with
           moderate doses of alcohol (*4). In fact, in a recent NHTSA study, the only statistically significant outcome
           associated with marijuana was speed reduction. (*5) A recent study of actual driving ability under the
           influence of cannabis - employing the same protocol used to test impairment-potential of medicinal drugs -
           evaluated the impact of placebo and three active THC doses in three driving trials, including one in
           high-density urban traffic. Dose-related impairment was observed in drivers' ability to maintain steady lateral
           position. However, even with the highest dose of THC, impairment was relatively minor - similar to that
           observed with blood-alcohol concentrations between 0.03 and 0.07 percent and many legal medications.
           Drivers under the influence of marijuana also tended to drive more slowly and approach other cars more
           cautiously While recognising some limitations of this study, the authors conclude that "THC is not a
           profoundly impairing drug." (*6) Refs: *1 : McBay AJ and Owens SM., "Marijuana and Driving", pp 257-63
           in L.S.Harris (ed) Problems of Drug Dependence 1980, Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office
           (1981): Teale, JD et al., "The Incidmnece of Cannabinoids in Fatally Impaired Drivers: An Investigation by
           Radioimmunoassay and High Pressure Liquid Chromatography," Journal of the Forensic Science Society 17:
           177-83 (1978) *2 Terhune, KW et al., The Incidence and Role of Drugs in Fatally Injured Drivers,
           Washington DC: Department of Transportation (1994) *3 Brookoff, D et al., "Testing Reckless Drivers for
           Cocaine and Marijuana", New England Journal of Medicine 331: 518-22 (1994) *4 Kv'alseth, TO, "Effects
           of Marijuana on Human Reaction Time and Motor Control", Perceptual and Motor Skills 45: 935-39 (1977):
           Hansteen, RW, et al, "Effects of Cannabis and Alcohol on Automobile Driving and Psychomotor Tracking,"
           Annals of New York Academy of Science 282: 240-56 (1976): Moskowitz, H et al., " Marijuana: Effects on
           Simulated Driving Performance," Accident Analysis and Prevention 8: 45-50 (1976); Moskowitz H et al.,
           "Visual Search Behaviour While Viewing Driving Scenes Under the Influence of Alcohol and Marijuana",
           Human Factors 18: 417-31 (1976) *5 Stein, AC et al., A Simulator Study of the Combined Effects of Alcohol
           and Marijuana on Driving Behavior-Phase II, Washington DC: Department of Transportation (1983)
           Cannabis makes you a better driver - more evidence Source: 1 The AGE 21 October 1998 pA5; 2.
           CANBERRA TIMES 21 October 1998 p4 The largest study ever done linking road accidents with drugs and
           alcohol has found drivers with cannabis in their blood were no more at risk than those who were drug-free. In
           fact, the findings by a pharmacology team from the University of Adelaide and Transport SA showed drivers
           who had smoked marijuana were marginally less likely to have an accident than those who were drug-free. A
           study spokesman, Dr Jason White, said the difference was not great enough to be statistically significant but
           could be explained by anecdotal evidence that marijuana smokers were more cautious and drove more slowly
           because of altered time perception. The study of 2,500 accidents, which matched the blood alcohol levels of
           injured drivers with details from police reports, found drug-free drivers caused the accidents in 53.5 per cent
           of cases. Injured drivers with a blood-alcohol concentration of more than 0.05 per cent were culpable in
           nearly 90 per cent of accidents they were involved in. Drivers with cannabis in their blood were less likely to
           cause an accident, with a culpability rate of 50.6 per cent. The study has policy implications for those who
           argue drug detection should be a new focus for road safety. Dr White said the study showed the importance
           of concentrating efforts on alcohol rather than other drugs. This information was posted by the library of The
           Alcohol and other Drugs Council of Australia (ADCA). Requests for copies of newsclips can be directed to
           the library by phone 02 62811002, fax 02 6282 7364 or e-mail library@adca.org.au. To subscribe to this
           listserv, send the message "subscribe update" (without the inverted commas) in the text field to
           majordomo@majordomo.netinfo.com.au with the subject field left empty.